CRITICISMS OF THE BIOPSYCHOSOCIAL MODEL IN SPINE CARE: CREATING AND THEN ATTACKING A STRAW PERSON Robert J. Gatchel, Ph.D., ABPP Nancy P. & John G. Penson Endowed Professor of Clinical Health Psychology Chair, Department of Psychology, College of Science The University of Texas at Arlington Dennis C. Turk, Ph.D. John and Emma Bonica Professor of Anesthesiology & Pain Research Department of Anesthesiology The University of Washington at Seattle Running head: Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model Address All Correspondence to: Robert J. Gatchel, Ph.D., ABPP Professor and Chair Department of Psychology, College of Science The University of Texas at Arlington 501 S. Nedderman Drive – Ste. 313 Arlington, TX 76019-0528 817-272-2541 (V) 817-272-2364 (F) gatchel@uta.edu Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 1 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 2 Mini Abstract A previous article in Spine 1 provided an erroneous and illogical critique of the biopsychosocial model. Evidence is used to refute concerns raised. We describe an evolving literature demonstrating the heuristic value of the model in developing effective assessment and treatment methods, and guiding research identifying etiological factors and prevention strategies. Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 2 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 3 ABSTRACT Study Design. Spine Update on some major misconceptions of the biopsychosocial model. Objective. To refute some of the erroneous beliefs about the weaknesses of the application of the biopsychosocial model to spine care. Summary and Background Data. Currently, the biopsychosocial model of illness is the most heuristic perspective in understanding the etiology, assessment, treatment, and prevention of pain-related disorders such as spinal pain and disability. Only the misuse of the biopsychosocial model by inappropriately trained health-care specialists decreases its maximum utility and validity. Methods. This is a point-by-point response to a previous article in Spine which inaccurately discussed some assumed limitations of the biopsychosocial model as related to spine care. It is also a more comprehensive review of the model and related clinical applications. Articles from the scientific literature are cited in refuting those assumed limitations. Results. The previous article in Spine provided a superficial, and often erroneous, review of the biopsychosocial model of illness. In providing a point-by-point refutation of that review, a number of important clarifications were delineated. For example, concerns raised about the sole reliance of self-report outcomes are shown to be unfounded. By definition, the “bio” “psycho” “social” underscores the important interactive contribution of factors in each of these defining domains, and requires their individual assessments. It was also erroneously stated that there was an inherent “disconnect” between physical pathology and self-report in this model. However, the richness of the biopsychosocial model is the recognition of the need to continue to understand the pathoanatomic and pathophysiological explanations of spinal disorders (the bio part of the equation), as well as the psychosocial factors that may also be important (the psychosocial part Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 3 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 4 of biopsychosocial). Other questions raised about the scientific status of the model, the effectiveness of treatments based on the model, and so forth. were also addressed. Conclusion. In agreement with the earlier Spine Update, the “...biopsychosocial model has been readily adapted to all aspects of spine care with many positive implications.” However, the author then raised some apparently major concerns. These concerns were shown to be unfounded. In point-of-fact, there is an ever-growing scientific literature demonstrating the heuristic value of this model in developing more effective assessment and treatment methods for spinal disorders, as well as guiding greater “cutting-edge” research on their etiologies and potential prevention techniques. KEY WORDS: biopsychosocial; spine care; interdisciplinary; functional restoration Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 4 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 5 In a recent article in Spine, Weiner1 provided a rather superficial, and we will argue erroneous, review of the rise of the biopsychosocial model of illness. In focusing on the heuristic value of the biopsychosocial model as it applies to spine care, Weiner goes on to extol its many virtues, strengths, and positive impact. However, he then goes on to suggest several concerns, and enumerates what he perceives as weaknesses of the model. The purpose of the present article is to delineate some of the flaws in Weiner’s analysis. We will address them in the order presented in Weiner’s treatise. 1 . Concerns about Reliance on Self-Report of Outcomes There is nothing inherent in the biopsychosocial model that limits outcome assessment to self-report. Some investigators may choose to limit the outcomes depending on the purpose of their study, but this is not an indictment of the model. By definition, the “bio” “psycho” “social” underscores the important contribution of factors in each of the three defining domains. Descriptions of the biopsychosocial model detail the importance of these three constituent domains or categories. Contrary to what Weiner asserts, the use of the biopsychosocial model does not limit outcome assessments to only self-reported psychosocial measures. Nothing could be farther from the truth! In fact, a substantial number of authors have emphasized repeatedly that there are three broad categories of measures—physical, psychosocial (including interpretations, affective state, behavior, and coping resources), and socioeconomic (including workplace factors, contextual demands, availability of wage replacement)—that should all be utilized to assess patients, in treatment planning, and as outcomes of clinical trials of spinal disorder patients (e.g.,2-5). Contrary to Weiner’s assertion, reliance on self-reported outcomes is not an indictment of the model, but rather a criticism that might be targeted toward some who only give “lip service” to the model but fail to adhere to the rationale of the fundamental basis Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 5 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 6 underlying the model. This basis emphasizes the importance of going beyond the Cartesian dichotomy of either physical or psychological, and integrating biological, psychological, and social factors as they interact in pain and subsequent disability. The three major categories (or biopsychosocial referents) comprising the biopsychosocial model, however, may not always display high concordance with one another when measuring a 2 construct such as spinal pain or disability. As Flores and colleagues have earlier noted in discussing the objectification of the measures of spine care outcomes: “...such less-than-perfect concordance among these behavioral referents of a construct...is not unique to the area of spinal disorders or rehabilitation medicine in general. It has long been noted ..., that self-report, overt behavior and physiological indices of behavior sometimes show low correlations among one another. Therefore, if one uses a self-report measure as a primary index of a construct and compares it to the overt behavioral or physiological index for the same construct, direct overlap cannot automatically be expected. In addition, two different self-report indices or physiological indices of the same construct may not be as highly correlated as one would desire. What has plagued the evaluation arena in general has been the lack of agreement in the wide variation of measures used to document a construct such as pain and disability, as well as changes in that construct. Therefore, the literature is replete with many different measurement techniques and tests of a construct such as function. However, the literature is beginning to demonstrate which measures ... appear to be most reliable and valid.” (page 1623). With the above quotation in mind, it is incumbent upon clinicians and investigators in the area of spine care to isolate what measurement or array of measurements are best used in Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 6 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 7 defining a construct, as well as documenting any changes in such a construct. There are a number of “assumption traps” that health care professionals need to avoid when considering what is the best outcome measurement to utilize. These traps are delineated below: 0 One cannot assume, on an a priori basis, that one outcome measure will necessarily be more valid or reliable than another. 6 It may be assumed that the more objective the measure, the more valid it will be; however, some outcomes are not easily assessed by objective measures. For example, pain, emotional distress, quality of sleep are primarily subjective. Even the putative behavioral expression of pain, so called pain behaviors, have to be validated against some “gold standard,” and the gold standard is self-report 7 . Even functional measures that rely on sophisticated performance-based equipment are dependent, at least to some extent, on patients’ willingness to perform at a maximal or optimal level. Since their behavior is voluntary, it may not be a perfect proxy for performance capability. Functional performance will be influenced by motivation, fear, understanding of instructions, as well as physical capacity. 0 No matter what the level of accuracy or sophistication of the mechanical device used in collecting physiologic measures, it is always the case that human inference must ultimately be used in the interpretation of the findings. Moreover, although physical examination might be viewed as more objective and more valid than patient self-reports, the inter-rater reliability of physical examination of such activities as range-of-motion is less than optimal8,9. 0 There is usually no easy answer to the question: “What is the best set of outcomes to use with a spinal pain patient?” because the question itself needs to be prefaced by a number of more specific inquiries such as: for what purpose is the assessment being performed (e.g., patient management; treatment planning; work capacity evaluation; surgical pre-screening purposes; evaluation of outcome in a clinical trial of an analgesic drug, surgery, or physical therapy)? How Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 7 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 8 will results on different outcomes be integrated? What if the outcomes on different measures are inconsistent? We agree that “outcomes assessment is not a done deal”1, p. 221. Indeed, in any area of medicine or science, there is an ever-changing “landscape” in the quality of the reliability (internal consistency; stability over time), validity (content; concurrent; predictive; sensitivity to change), as well as the fidelity of measurement techniques, and their interpretability. Nevertheless, there is no doubt that psychosocial factors have consistently been found to be more predictive than physical ones in accounting for chronic low back pain (e.g.,10-13 ). Although such factors do not account for all the variance, they account for substantially more of the variance than physical variables. This is not to say that a health care provider should ignore such important physical variables, but should include them in a more comprehensive biopsychosocial perspective. The data supporting the predictive power of psychosocial variables support and thus validate the biopsychosocial model, not countermand it (e.g. ,14-17) . The answer to which array of variables “carry equal, if not primary weight” will vary from patient to patient, as well as across diagnostic entities, and possibly across time. 2. The Disconnection between Physical Pathology and Self-report Weiner 1 cautions us that “the history of medicine, however, is filled with tales of diseases with insufficiently understood etiologic pathology and poor outcomes of treatment being inappropriately correlated (in its worst forms, etiologically/causally) with psychosocial phenomena” (page 221). He goes on to suggest that history tells us that embracing a biopsychosocial orientation will actually hamper the development of a better understanding of the etiology of many diseases. The examples he provides are spurious. For instance, he asserts that the Type A behavior pattern has impeded our understanding and treatment of coronary artery Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 8 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 9 disease. However, quite to the contrary, the identification of the Type A behavior pattern and its relationship to stress opened up whole new pathways of investigation that unequivocally demonstrated that the psychosocial construct of “stress” was a major risk factor for certain forms of coronary heart disease (e.g.,18) , albeit not necessarily all forms. There are now even textbooks that focus on such important psychosocial factors in the field of cardiology (e.g., Contributions Toward Evidence-Based Psychocardiology19). There are also new journals that focus on biopsychosocial factors and medicine (e.g., BioPsychoSocial Medicine; http://www.bpsmedicine.com/content). There has also been a number of other areas in medicine where this biopsychosocial approach has demonstrated the importance of considering psychosocial and lifestyle factors in common illnesses such as diabetes mellitus, hypertension, asthma, and certain types of gastrointestinal disorders, just to name a few (e.g.,20). Weiner seems to want to return to the dated and discredited somatogenic-psychogenic dichotomy. This is exactly what the biopsychosocial model was created to replace. All people have different genetic compositions, prior learning histories, physical experiences, and they live in a social context. This way of thinking helps us to understand the diversity of responses to what might appear to be objectively the same pathophysiology. The biopsychosocial orientation had its initial roots in the work of Engel21, who cogently pointed out some of the severe limitations of the traditional biomedical theories of disease and causation, and posed the biopsychosocial model as a needed extension. It is not that the medical model is wrong, but rather it is incomplete. We do not have to make an “either-or” decision. The richness and heuristic value of the biopsychosocial model is the recognition that we need to continue to understand the pathoanatomic and pathophysiological explanations of low back pain (the bio part of the equation), as well as the psychosocial factors Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 9 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 10 that may be important in low back pain (the psychosocial part of biopsychosocial). Indeed, as a recent comprehensive review of the great advances in basic neuroscience processes of pain, as well as the development of new technologies such as brain imaging, new insights into the etiology of pain conditions such as low back pain is advancing significantly (e.g.,22). Finally, because there are currently no permanent “cures” (and none imminently on the 23 horizon) for the majority of spinal problems , as well as many other chronic diseases (e.g., hypertension, diabetes, asthma, post-stroke syndrome, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease), one needs to move away from an exclusively curative approach to a more comprehensive rehabilitative-management approach to dealing with these chronic medical illnesses. Thus, for example, the treatment of hypertension involves not only the administration of medication, but also lifestyle issues such as diet, exercise, smoking cessation, and so forth, all of which can be affected by psychosocial factors such as compliance, culture, socioeconomic status, coping, among others. The same can be said for spinal disorders where individual differences in psychosocial factors can significantly affect the efficacy of traditional treatments such as spine surgery 24, and can contribute to the success of rehabilitation. 3. The Scientific Status of the Biopsychosocial Model Weiner states that “a key ingredient to scientific theories is that they are testable/falsifiable” (page 22 1), and that a biomedical model was effective in hypothesizing that certain somatic pathophysiological events may be responsible for low back pain, and could be objectively tested. However, as noted earlier, we contend that, even though this is possible, the biomedical model has failed to this day to account for the majority of the variance required to fully understand the etiology, progression, and the effective treatment of low back pain. As we noted previously, substantial amounts of variance in the evolution and maintenance of disability Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 10 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 1 1 following back injury are predicted by psychosocial factors, proportionately more variance than can be accounted for by physical variables. These data illustrate that the biopsychosocial model is, indeed, a testable and, yes, falsifiable model. Failure of psychosocial factors to predict, or predict only a small proportion of the variance, might lead to the conclusion that the model was inadequate and thereby challenge the biopsychosocial model. There is a legitimate concern that failure of psychosocial factors to predict might lead to a conclusion that the wrong factors had been studied calling for more research into the potentially infinite number of psychological and social variables that might be important. However, the demonstration that specific psychosocial factors do predict significant amounts of variance makes this concern moot. Furthermore, the biopsychosocial model has led to the development of a very effective interdisciplinary approach to the treatment of chronic low back pain. Unlike previous biomedically based approaches that emphasized monotherapies, such as surgery, pharmacotherapy, invasive procedures, and so forth alone, none of which have been proven unequivocally to be effective for all patients; the interdisciplinary approach has been repeatedly documented to be the most clinically effective and cost-effective approach for chronic noncancer pain such as chronic low back pain (cf,25-28) . Failure of such treatments would lead to disconfirmation of the model on which they were predicated, namely, the biopsychosocial model. Once again, the ability of such outcome studies to demonstrate, confirm, or falsify the biopsychosocial model argues against Weiner’s criticism that the model is not falsifiable. 4. The Biopsychosocial Model Is Based on the Premise that Illness Is a Complex Synthesis of Biological, Cognitive, Psychological, and Social Factors Contrary to Weiner’s 1 statement that “Philosophers have fretted for millennia over mind/body interaction and implementing models based on debatable premises must itself be Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 11 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 12 subjected to debate” argument (p. 222), the status of the biopsychosocial model has progressed passed the overly simplistic mind/body dichotomy that was the major underpinning of the reductionist biomedical approach. The fact that a biopsychosocial model requires a better understanding of the complex interaction of a number of factors does not make it untenable. To the contrary, the fact that a complex goal is difficult to achieve should not prompt us to abandon the pursuit of that goal for the seduction of a more immediate, albeit “quick fix,” One that is only minimally effective. We are still at the infancy stage in developing complex solutions for complex problems. 5. The Outcomes of Treatments for Persistent Spinal Pain Based on the Biopsychosocial Model Are Just Now Being Studied, and there Is Conflicting Evidence to Date of Their Effectiveness in Decreasing Pain and Improving Function Is there any treatment for back pain for which this statement would not be true?23 As noted above, the review of the pain literature has unequivocally demonstrated the therapeutic effectiveness of the interdisciplinary approach to chronic pain (e.g. ,25,28). In fact, there is an extensive literature demonstrating the therapeutic effectiveness of a biopsychosocial model-based functional restoration program for chronic low back pain. The results of such programs (e.g. ,29-31) have demonstrated significant positive socioeconomic outcomes (such as return-to-work, decreased surgical rates, resolution of outstanding legal and medical issues) in chronically disabled patients with spinal disorders in both one-year as well as two-year follow-up studies. These results have been independently replicated by Hazard et al. and Patrick et al. in the United States, as well as in RCTs conducted by Bendix et al. in Denmark, Hildebrandt et al. in Germany, Corey et al. in Canada, Jousset et al. in France, and Shirado et al. in Japan32-38. The observation that different clinical treatment teams, functioning in different states and different Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 12 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 13 countries, with markedly different economic and social conditions and workers’ compensation systems, produce comparable positive outcome results speaks highly for the robustness of the research findings and utility, as well as the fidelity, of this approach to pain management in occupational settings for patients with low back pain. Moreover, comprehensive reviews of the effectiveness of interdisciplinary pain management programs in general have further documented the clinical utility of the biopsychosocial model. 26,39 Weiner 1 seems to have totally disregarded the outcomes of such clinical trials. In fact, Rainville, Kim and Katz 40 have recently noted that, during the past 20 years since Mayer and Gatchel first introduced the functional restoration model: “...This treatment model has received considerable study worldwide, and it is generally agreed that it is superior to standard care for reducing work absence in patients with chronic low back pain. Additionally, the concepts underlying functional restoration have been found to be highly relevant to patients with chronic low back pain, medical providers, and disability systems and continue to gain acceptance and integration into the care of patients throughout the industrialized world.” (p. 18). 6. The Concern of the Ubiquity of Biopsychosocial “Pathology” The fact raised that “A recent study in nonpatients demonstrated that 49% of ‘healthy’ people demonstrated biopsychosocial dysfunction on standardized questionnaires 41”( p. 222) reinforces the notion that many patients “bring with them” unique characteristics that often need to be considered in assessment and treatment planning. There is no question that patients bring with them the premorbid histories when they seek treatment for this back pain. The 49% figure cited does not necessarily mean that all these people are in urgent need of any form of treatment. Similarly, the base rates of major psychopathology, such as major depressive disorder (17. 1 %), anxiety disorders such as panic disorder (3.5%), and substance abuse disorders (26.6%) are quite Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 13 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 14 high in the general “normal” population who are not psychiatric patients 42. These individuals have learned to cope with these maladaptive symptoms and behaviors. However, as proposed by many 5,43-45, a diathesis-stress perspective is emerging as the dominant one to understand why many “biopsychosocial dysfunctions” or “psychopathology” may be significant for some individuals and not others. The diathesis-stress perspective assumes that all patients (spinal care patients or patients with other medical diagnoses) “bring with them” certain pre-existing and predisposing biopsychosocial characteristics (genetics and history that create a diathesis) that can then be exacerbated by the stress of attempting to cope with a painful or chronic condition that negatively affects activities of daily living. Indeed, the relationship between stress and the exacerbation of mental health problems has long been documented in the scientific literature 46 This not to say that such predisposing factors make illnesses such as chronic spinal pain a psychogenic disorder and that “it is all in the patient’s head.” Rather, it emphasizes that this chronic problem may represent a complex interaction between physical factors and psychosocioeconomic variables that all need to be effectively managed to ensure therapeutic success. Of course, this means that greater progress and awareness need to be made in the more comprehensive diagnostic process. 7. A Final Concern Regarding the Biopsychosocial Model Finally, the concern raised by Weiner that the biopsychosocial model “encourages the further medicalization of the patient” (p. 222) is quite perplexing as it turns the entire model on its head. We believe that Weiner’s assertion is illogical and misguided. As we have reviewed above, an illness such as intractable chronic low back pain is not conceptualized as purely a nociceptive problem that simply requires a structural “fix of some broken body part”, but one that results from the interaction of biopsychosocial factors that need to be carefully assessed in Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 14 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 15 order to then “customize” treatment to the specific needs of the patient – physical, psychosocial, and behavioral. One does not mindlessly prescribe a series of evaluations until the “right one” shows some measured, unique pathophysiology that totally accounts for the syndrome that is then surgically or medically “remediated.” Rather, a step-wise approach is used to progressively establish a comprehensive understanding of the whole problem. The best method to achieve this is to use an integrated team of health care professionals working under one roof, all trained in the biopsychosocial model and, thus, “speaking one language” when evaluating and treating patients. Weiner1 is quite correct in pointing out the often inappropriate uses of a biopsychosocial “healthcare team” composed of multiple professionals working independently and “handing off” the patient from one to another in a non-integrated fashion. This represents the glaring, iconoclastic, misuse of the biopsychosocial model by inappropriately trained health care specialists. Moreover, third-party payers encourage such a non-integrated approach because of cost-saving reasons. 26 When services are “carved out” to different professionals who are not part of the same health-care team, then therapeutic outcomes are less effective! 47-49 We believe that Weiner’s article unfortunately creates and then attacks a straw person. As we have enumerated, his critique of the biopsychosocial model is superficial at best, and misleading at least. There is no question that the model can be misused by some, but inappropriate behavior does not undermine the power, utility, and hence importance of the model per se. “Science for its part will test relentlessly every assumption about the human condition ... Old beliefs die hard even when demonstrably false.” E.O. Wilson Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 15 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 16 REFERENCES 1 . Weiner BK. The biopsychosocial model and spine care. Spine 2008;33:219-23. 2. Flores L, Gatchel RJ, Polatin PB. Objectification of functional improvement after nonoperative care. Spine 1997;22:1622-33. 3. Gatchel RJ ed. Compendium of Outcome Instruments. 2nd ed. LeGrange, IL: North American Spine Society, 2006. 4. Waddell G. The Back Pain Revolution. Edinburgh, UK: Churchill Livingstone, 1999. 5. Gatchel RJ. Clinical Essentials of Pain Management. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2005. 6. Gatchel RJ, Kishino ND, Strezak A. The importance of outcome assessment in orthopaedics: An overview. In Spivak JM, Connolly PJ eds. Orthopaedic Knowledge Update: Spine. Chicago, IL: American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons, 2006. 7. Keefe FJ, Block AR. Development of an observation method for assessing pain behavior in chronic low back pain patients. Behavior Therapy 1982; 13:363-75. 8. Hunt DG, Zuberbier OA, Kozolowski AJ, et al. Reliability of the lumbar flexion, lumbar extension and passive straight leg raise test in normal populations embedded within a complete physical examination. Spine 2001;26. 9. Nitschke JE, Nattrass CL, Disler PB, et al. Reliability of the American Medical Association Guides' model for measuring spinal range of motion. Spine 1999;24:262-8. 10. Carragee EJ, Alamin TF, Miller JL, et al. Discographic, MRI and psychosocial determinants of low back pain disability and remission: A prospective study in subjects with benign persistent back pain. The Spine Journal 2005;5:24-35. Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 16 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 17 11. Jarvik JG, Hollingworth W, Heaggerty PJ, et al. Three-year incidence of low back pain in an initially asymptomatic cohort. Clinical and imaging risk factors. Spine 2005;30:1541-8. 12. Pruitt SD, Von Korff M. Improving the Management of Low Back Pain: A Paradigm Shift for Primary Care. In Turk DC, Gatchel RJ eds. Psychological Approaches to Pain Management: A Practitioner's Handbook. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, 2002. 13. Waddell G. Preventing incapacity in people with musculoskeletal disorders. Br Med Bull 2006;78:55-69. 14. Turner JA, Franklin G, Turk D. Predictors of chronic disability in injured workers: A systematic literature synthesis. Am J Ind Med 2000; 3 8. 15. Crook J, Milner R, Schultz IZ, et al. Determinants of occupational disability following a low back injury: A critical review of the literature. Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation 2002; 12:277-94. 16. Brage S, Sandanga I, Nyga^^ ^^. Emotional distress as a predictor for low back disability. Spine 2007;32:269-74. 17. van Geen JW, Edelaar MJA, Janssen M, et al. The long-term effect of multidisciplinary back training. Spine 2007;32:249-55. 18. Baum A, Gatchel RJ, Krantz DS eds. An Introduction to Health Psychology. 3rd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1997. 19. Jordon J, Barde B, Zeiher AM. Contributions Toward Evidence-Based Psychocardiology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press, 2008. 20. Gatchel RJ, Oordt MS. Clinical Health Psychology and Primary Care: Practical Advice and Clinical Guidance for Successful Collaboration. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2003. Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 17 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 18 21. Engel GL. The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science 1977;196:129-36. 22. Gatchel RJ, Peng Y, Peters ML, et al. The Biopsychosocial Approach to Chronic Pain: Scientific Advances and Future Directions. Psychological Bulletin 2007; 13 3:5 81-624. 23. Martin BI, Deyo RA, Mirza SK, et al. Expenditures and health status among adults with back and neck problems. JAMA 2008;299:656-64. 24. Block AR, Gatchel RJ, Deardorff W, et al. The Psychology of Spine Surgery. Washington: American Psychological Association, 2003. 25. Chou R, Huffman LH. Nonpharmacologic therapies for acute and chronic low back pain: A review of the evidence for an American Pain Society/American College of Physicians Clinical Practice Guideline. Ann Intern Med 2007; 147:492-504. 26. Gatchel RJ, Okifuji A. Evidence-based scientific data documenting the treatment- and cost-effectiveness of comprehensive pain programs for chronic nonmalignant pain. Journal of Pain 2006;7:779-93. 27. Turk DC, Swanson DC, Gatchel RJ. Predicting opioid misuse by chronic pain patients: A systematic review and literature synthesis. Clin J Pain In Press. 28. Guzman J, Esmail R, Karjalinen K, et al. Multidisciplinary rehabilitation for chronic low back pain: Systematic review. Br Med J 2001;322:1511-6. 29. Hazard RG, Fenwick JW, Kalisch SM, et al. Functional restoration with behavioral support. A one-year prospective study of patients with chronic low-back pain. Spine 1989;14:157-61. 30. Mayer T, Smith S, Keeley J, et al. Quantification of lumbar function. Part 2: Sagittal plane trunk strength in chronic low back pain patients. Spine 1985; 10:765-72. Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 18 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 19 31. Mayer TG, Gatchel RJ, Mayer H, et al. A prospective two-year study of functional restoration in industrial low back injury. JAMA 1987;258:1181-2. 32. Bendix T, Bendix A. Different training programs for chronic low back pain--A randomized, blinded one-year follow-up study. International Society for the Study of the Lumbar Spine. Seattle, 1994. 33. Bendix AE, Bendix T, Vaegter K, et al. Multidisciplinary intensive treatment for chronic low back pain: A randomized, prospective study. Cleve Clin J Med 1996;63:62-9. 34. Bendix T, Bendix AF, Lund C, et al. Comparison of three intensive programs for chronic low back pain patients: A prospective, randomized, observer-blinded study with one-year follow-up. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 1997;29:81-9. 35. Hildebrandt J, Pfingsten M, Saur P, et al. Prediction of success from a multidisciplinary treatment program for chronic low back pain. Spine 1997;22:990-1001. 36. Corey DT, Koepfler LE, Etlin D, et al. A limited functional restoration program for injured workers: A randomized trial. Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation 1996;6:239-49. 37. Jousset N, Fanello S, Bontoux L, et al. Effects of functional restoration versus 3 hours per week physical therapy: A randomized controlled study. Spine 2004;29:487-93. 38. Shirado O, Ito T, Kikumoto T, et al. A novel back school using a multidisciplinary team approach featuring quantitative functional evaluation and therapeutic exercises for patients with chronic low back pain. Spine 2005;30:1219-25. 39. Chou R, Qaseem A, Snow V, et al. Guidelines for the diagnosis and treatment of low back pain: A joint clinical practice guideline from the American College of Physicians and the American Pain Society. Ann Intern Med 2007; 147:478-9 1. Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 19 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 20 40. Rainville J, Kim RS, Katz JN. A review of 1985 Volvo Award winner in clinical science: Objective assessment of spine function following industrial injury: A prospective study with comparison group and 1-year follow-up. Spine 2007;32:2031-4. 41. Cassidy JD, Carroll LJ, Cote P, et al. Does multidisciplinary rehabilitation benefit whiplash recovery? Spine 2007;32:126-3 1. 42. Dersh J, Mayer T, Gatchel R, et al. Do psychiatric disorders affect functional restoration outcomes in chronic disabling occupational spinal disorders? Spine 2007; 3 2:1045 -5 1. 43. Banks SM, Kerns RD. Explaining the high rates of depression in chronic pain: A diathesis-stress framework. Psychological Bulletin 1996; 119:95-110. 44. Weisberg JN, Vittengle JR, Clark LA, et al. Personality and pain: Summary and future directions. In Gatchel RJ, Weisberg JN eds. Personality Characteristics of Patients with Pain. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 2000. 45. Turk DC. A diathesis-stress model of chronic pain and disability following traumatic injury. Pain Research & Management 2002;7:9-20. 46. Barrett JF, Rose RM, Klerman GL eds. Stress and Mental Disordered. New York: Raven Press, 1979. 47. Gatchel RJ, Noe C, Gajraj N, et al. The negative impact on an interdisciplinary pain management program of insurance "treatment carve out" practices. Journal of Workers Compensation 2001;10:50-63. 48. Keel P, Wittig R, Deutschman R, et al. Effectiveness of in-patient rehabilitation for sub-chronic and chronic low back pain by a integrative group treatment program. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 1998;30:211-9. Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 20 Criticisms of the Biopsychosocial Model 21 49. Robbins H, Gatchel RJ, Noe C, et al. A prospective one-year outcome study of interdisciplinary chronic pain management: Compromising its efficacy by managed care policies. Anesth Analg 2003;97:156-62. Manuscripts\StrawPersonR1-rjg-dct.804\August 10, 2010 21